The Undoing Project: A Friendship that Changed the World(29)



Michigan required that all PhD students in psychology pass a proficiency test in two foreign languages. Weirdly, the university didn’t count Hebrew as a foreign language but accepted mathematics. Though entirely self-taught in mathematics, Amos chose math as one of his languages and passed the test. For his second language he picked French. The test was to translate three pages from a book in the language: The student chose the book, and the tester chose the pages to translate. Amos went to the library and dug out a French math textbook with nothing but equations in it. “It might have had the word donc in it,” said Amos’s roommate Mel Guyer. The University of Michigan declared Amos Tversky proficient in French.

Amos wanted to explore how people made decisions. To do this he required subjects who were both captive and poor enough that they would respond to the tiny financial incentives he could offer. He found them in the maximum security wing of the Jackson State Prison, near Ann Arbor. Amos offered the inmates—though only those with IQs over 100—different gambles, involving candy and cigarettes. Both functioned in the jail as currency, and everyone knew what they were worth—a pack of cigarettes and a sack of candy at the prison store each cost 30 cents, or about a week’s salary. The inmates could either take the gamble or sell the right to take the gamble to Amos—that is, receive a sure payout.

As it turned out, the Jackson Prison inmates choosing between gambles had a lot in common with Kenneth May’s students when they chose between spouses: After they had said they preferred A to B and B to C, they could be induced to prefer C to A. Even when you asked them up front whether they would ever chose C over A and they insisted they would never do such a thing, they did it. Some thought Amos must be playing a trick on the inmates, but he wasn’t. “He didn’t trick the prisoners into violating transitivity,” says Michigan professor Rich Gonzalez. “He used a process much like the old saying about the frog in the pot of boiling water. As the temperature increases slowly, the frog can’t detect it. Obviously the frog can detect 90 degrees versus 200 degrees, but not increments of a single degree. In some of our biological systems we are equipped to detect big differences; in others, small ones—say, a tickle versus a poke. If people can’t detect small differences, Amos figured, they might violate transitivity.”

Clearly people had trouble detecting small differences. Prison inmates and Harvard students, on whom Amos also ran tests. He wrote a paper about his experiments in which he showed how one might even predict when people would be intransitive. And yet . . . he didn’t read much into this. Rather than draw some grand conclusions about the inadequacy of existing assumptions about human rationality, he pulled himself up short. “Is this behavior irrational?” he wrote. “We tend to doubt it. . . . When faced with complex multidimensional alternatives, such as job offers, gambles or [political] candidates, it is extremely difficult to utilize properly all the available information.” It wasn’t that people actually preferred A to B and B to C and then turned around and preferred C to A. It was that it was sometimes very hard to understand the differences. Amos didn’t think that the real world was as likely to fool people into contradicting themselves as were the experiments he had designed.

The man whose work had pulled Amos to Michigan, Ward Edwards, turned out to be more appealing to Amos on the page than in the flesh. After Johns Hopkins fired him, Edwards found a place in Michigan, but his position was insecure, and so was he. When students arrived to work with him, he gave each of them a pompous little lecture—they called it the “key” lecture. Edwards would hold up the key to the door of the small house that served as his lab and tell the student what an honor it was for him to be entrusted with the key and, by extension, an association with Edwards. “You got this key along with the speech,” says Paul Slovic. “The meaning of the key, the symbol of the key—it was all a little weird. Usually someone just gives you a key and tells you to make sure you lock the door when you leave.”

Edwards hosted a party at his house for some visiting scholar—and charged his guests for the beer. He sent Amos out to do research for him and then withheld his expenses until Amos put up a fight. He insisted that any work Amos did in his lab was at least in part the property of Ward Edwards, and thus any paper that Amos wrote should also have Ward Edwards’s name on it. Amos liked to say that stinginess was contagious and so was generosity, and since behaving generously made you happier than behaving stingily, you should avoid stingy people and spend your time only with generous ones. He paid attention to what Edwards was up to without paying a lot of attention to Edwards himself.

The University of Michigan was then, as it is now, home to the world’s largest department of psychology. There were others in it thinking about decision making, and Amos found himself drawn to one of them, Clyde Coombs. Coombs drew a distinction between the sorts of decisions in which more was better, and more subtle decisions. For instance, other things being equal, just about everyone would decide to take more money rather than less, and to accept less pain rather than more. What interested Coombs were the fuzzier decisions. How does a person decide where to live, or whom to marry, or, for that matter, which jam to buy? The giant food company General Mills had hired Coombs in hopes that he might create for them tools to measure their customers’ feelings about their products. But how do you measure the strength of a person’s feelings for Cheerios? What kind of scale do you use? A person might be twice as tall as another person, but might he like something twice as much? One place might be ten degrees hotter than another place; could one person’s feelings for a breakfast cereal be ten degrees hotter than another’s? To predict what people would decide, you had to be able to measure their preferences: but how?

Michael Lewis's Books